|
Some articles have not been moved to our new site yet.
As a result you have been redirected to our old site.
If you wish to return to our new site - click here.
Ecstasy and Driving Ability
1 Overview
- Effects of ecstasy
1.1 Ecstasy
and related drugs (phenethylamines) are stimulant
drugs chemically related to amphetamine, but with
other psychoactive (entheogenic) properties affecting
mood. The main physiological action is to increase
the release of serotonin (5-hydroxy tryptamine) and
dopamine in the brain, changing the mood of the user
by altering the processing of information. The half-life
of ecstasy (MDMA) in blood is around 6 hours. In general
the effects are similar to, but more pronouced than,
those of Prozac or similar antidepressants which block
reuptake of serotonin thus raising the levels. It
could be said that they achieve the same effects by
ecstasy "turning on the taps", and Prozac
"putting in the plug".
1.2 Physiological effects are
similar to other stimulants - increased alertness
and physical energy, raised body temperature (in many
cases dangerously so), and dehydration. Liver and
kidney damage has been reported. Most deaths have
occurred due to overheating (heat stroke).
1.3 The main short-term psychological
effects are reported by Leister & Grob as
"altered time perception (90%), increased
ability to interact with or be open with others (85%),
decreased defensiveness (80%), decreased fear (65%),
decreased sense of separation or alienation (60%),
changes in visual perception (55%), increased awareness
of emotions (50%), decreased aggression (50%), speech
changes (45%), awareness of unconscious memories (40%),
decreased obsessiveness (40% and cognitive changes
(40%)." Medium-term changes (up to a week)
included "decreased sleep (40%), decreased
appetite (30%), increased sensitivity to emotions
(25%), decreased ability or desire to perform mental
or physical tasks (20%), increased ability to interact
with or be open with others (20%), and decreased defensiveness
(20%)." Longer-term effects (more than one
week) included "improved social-interpersonal
functioning (50%), changes in religious orientation
or practice (46%), changed values or life priorities
(45%), improved occupational functioning (40%), increased
ability to interact with or be open with others (35%),
decreased defensiveness (30%), changes in ego boundaries
(30%), decreased desire to use alcohol (25% )and decreased
fear (20%)" Other researchers have identified
problems with memory and judgement, or increased sensitivity
to touch and sound.
1.4 Short-term side effects include
dry mouth, clenching or grinding of teeth, reduced
appetite, weight loss, nystagmus, twitches, nausea
and cramp. Adverse effects on mood are attributed
by Saunders to the increased vulnerability of users
when under the influence, due to increased openness
and reduced defensiveness, such that insights may
bring unbearable truths, and candid disclosures may
be regretted. Adverse psychological effects include
sleep disturbance, depression and paranoia, and exhaustion
after use. Tolerance to the effects of ecstasy develops
rapidly, such that the extreme euphoric effects tend
to be limited to the first few occasions of use, and
users may take increasing quantities in attempting
to achieve a desired level of intoxication. Regular
users can experience medium term depression when not
under the influence, which tends to reinforce ecstasy-seeking
behaviour.
1.5 Evidence of damage to serotonergic
neurons, and lower serotonin levels among users and
laboratory animals, provides a physiological model
predicting long-term depressive illness in many chronic
users. The neuronal damage also explains why toleranceoccurs,
as no matter how much stimulation they receive, the
fewer remaining neurons can only secrete a limited
amount of serotonin.
1.6 The effects of ecstasy typically
last for 4-6 hours, although where high or multiple
doses are taken the effects can be prolonged and sustained
in a pattern of "chronic binge use".
2. Laboratory
Studies of Psychomotor performance
2.1 Laboratory tests are frequently
used to assess the effects of drugs on performance
of psychomotor tests (dexterity, reaction time, tracking
etc) and cognitive function (mental ability and judgement).
The effects frequently differ between naive users
of a drug and experienced users who have developed
tolerance.
2.2 Vollenweider
et al studying the effects of a dose of 1.7mg/kg in
13x healthy ecstasy-na"ve volunteers, found "MDMA
produced an effective state of enhanced mood, well-being,
and increased emotional sensitiveness, little anxiety,
but no hallucinations or panic reactions. Mild depersonalization
and derealization phenomena occurred together with
moderate thought disorder, first signs of loss of
body control, and alterations in the meaning of percepts.
Subjects also displayed changes in the sense of space
and time, heightened sensory awareness, and increased
psychomotor drive. MDMA did not impair selective attention
as measured by the Stroop test. MDMA increased blood
pressure moderately, with the exception of one subject
who showed a transient hypertensive reaction.
"
2.3 Hermle
et al found ecstasy "produced
a partially controllable state of enhanced insight,
empathy, and peaceful feelings. All subjects displayed
a general stimulation with increased psychomotor drive,
logorrhea, and facilitation of communication. "
Mas et al noted significant increases in resting heart
rate (+30bpm) and blood pressure (+40mmHg) following
administration of single doses of 75-125mg MDMA in
healthy volunteers.
2.4 Verkes
et al assessed reaction time, direct recall, and recognition
in two groups, all from the same subculture, of 21
males with moderate and heavy recreational MDMA use,
and a control group of 20 males without use of MDMA,
finding "Ecstasy users showed
a broad pattern of statistically significant, but
clinically small, impairment of memory and prolonged
reaction times. Heavy users were affected stronger
than moderate users."
2.5 Studying
rhesus monkeys, Frederic et al found tolerance developed
to the behavioural and psychomotor effects of repeated
doses of ecstasy, although baseline values remained
unaltered on the "operant test
battery (OTB) designed to model aspects of time estimation,
short-term memory, motivation, learning, and color
and position discrimination." Taffe
et al found "Behavioral performance
was disrupted during acute MDMA treatment but returned
to baseline within one week following treatment."
3 Ecstasy
and Cognitive Function
3.1 Morgan
found chronic ecstasy users to show significant memory
deficits, and commented "...deficits
in memory performance in recreational ecstasy users
are primarily associated with past exposure to ecstasy,
rather than with the other legal and illicit drugs
consumed by these individuals, and are consistent
with reduced serotonergic modulation of mnemonic function
as a result of long-term neurotoxic effects of MDMA
in humans." Bolla et al found "Abstinent
MDMA users have impairment in verbal and visual memory.
The extent of memory impairment correlates with the
degree of MDMA exposure"
3.2 Parrott
& Lasky compared the performances of regular and
novice ecstasy users in a club environment, together
with other drug, but not ecstasy, using controls,
concluding "Cognitive performance
on both tasks (verbal recall, visual scanning) was
significantly reduced on-MDMA. Memory recall was also
significantly impaired in drug-free MDMA users, with
regular ecstasy users displaying the worst memory
scores at every test session. This agrees with previous
findings of memory impairments in drug-free ecstasy
users."
3.3 Gerra
et al, studying human volunteers, reported "Dysphoria
and mood changes were exhibited in seven individuals,
tiredness in five and sensation-seeking behaviour
in twelve at the clinical evaluation. Significantly
higher scores were found in MDMA individuals than
in control individuals for MMPI subscale for Depression,
for Buss Durkee Hostility Inventory direct and guilt
subscales, for Hamilton Depression Rating Scale and
for novelty-seeking Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire
subscale. "
3.4 Rodgers
studied cognitive function in 15 regular users of
ecstasy; 15 regular users of cannabis who had never
taken ecstasy and 15 control subjects who had never
taken any illicit substances, and found "Performance
was similar across all three groups for measures of
visual reaction time, auditory reaction time, complex
reaction time, visual memory and attention and concentration.
Significant impairment was found on measures of verbal
memory in both cannabis users and ecstasy users. A
significant impairment in performance was found on
measures of delayed memory for the ecstasy users compared
to both the cannabis group and the control group.
Despite these findings, no differences in subjective
ratings of cognitive failures were found between the
groups."
3.5 Curran
et al noted the tendency of ecstasy users to suffer
mood swings, with elation at weekends and depression
during the week, and also reported "The
MDMA group showed significant impairments on an attentional/working
memory task" Krystal et al noted in
a study of 9 chronic ecstasy users "a
pattern of mild-to-moderate impairment was observed
on both the Initial and Delayed Paragraph Tests of
the Wechsler Memory Scale; eight of the subjects had
at least mild impairment on at least one test in the
neuropsychological battery."
3.6 Gouzoulis-Mayfrank
et al studied abstinent former recreational users
of ecstasy, compared to cannabis users and non-drug
using controls, and reported "Ecstasy
users were unimpaired in simple tests of attention
(alertness). However, they performed worse than one
or both control groups in the more complex tests of
attention, in memory and learning tasks, and in tasks
reflecting aspects of general intelligence. Heavier
ecstasy and heavier cannabis use were associated with
poorer performance in the group of ecstasy users.
By contrast, the cannabis users did not differ significantly
in their performance from the non-users."
3.7 Parrott
et al studied two abstinent groups of "novice"
and "experienced" ecstasy users, compared
with non-using controls on a computerised battery
of tests, and found "Performance
on the response speed and vigilance measures (simple
reaction time, choice reaction time, number vigilance),
was similar across the three subgroups. However on
immediate word recall and delayed word recall, both
groups of MDMA users recalled significantly less words
than controls."
3.8 Studying
rats, Marston et al reported "MDMA
exposure elicits a classical 5-HT syndrome. In the
long-term, exposure results in 5-HT neurotoxicity
and a lasting cognitive impairment. These results
have significant implications for the prediction that
use of MDMA in humans could have deleterious long-term
neuropsychological/psychiatric consequences."
Also with rats, Robinson et al reported "Partial
depletion of neocortical serotonin (72.6%) did not
produce deficits on a variety of behavioral tests,
including a place navigation learning-set task, skilled
forelimb use, or the ability to make complex judgements
regarding the stimulus properties of food in a foraging
situation, and neither did additional cholinergic
blockade. MDMA-pretreated rats had a mild impairment
in rapidly developing an efficient search strategy
in the place navigation task, but once the goal was
located, MDMA pretreated rats performed at control
levels and showed no deficits in memory for spatial
location." In pigeons, LeSage et al
found MDMA impaired accuracy in a delayed-matching
task, but that tolerance developed with repeated administration.
4 Driving
Performance
4.1 There have been very few studies
specifically investigating the effect of ecstasy on
driving ability, although there is a wealth of evidence
as to the psychomotor effects of stimulant drugs in
general (e.g. amphetamine and cocaine), which suggests
low doses may enhance performance (more alert, vigilant,
quicker reaction times) whereas high doses may impair
judgement (overconfidence, more aggressive driving,
paranoia). However ecstasy are considered to be qualitatively
different from those of either amphetamine or hallucinations
such as LSD, and the class of drugs has been designated
"entactogens"
4.2 In
a learned review, Morland reported ecstasy to cause
effects including "euphoria,
central nervous stimulation, and feeling of closeness
to mild hallucinations, impairment of cognition and
co-ordination and further to serious reactions like
agitation, disturbed and bizarre behaviour, and possibly
psychosis." and noted: "It
has been assumed that the risk of being involved in
fatalities and accidents during the state of MDMA
influence is increased, but this possible risk increase
has so far not been determined. Observations of the
prevalence of MDMA involvement in cases of reckless
driving and the MDMA blood concentrations measured
indicate a risk increase comparable to that observed
after use of amphetamines." Moeller
et al reported ecstasy levels in impaired drivers,
and Crifasi et al reported a case history of a fatal
road accident attributed to MDMA use by a na"ve
user, although I am currently unaware of any evidence
as to causation or circumstances of that accident.
4.3 Giroud
et al reported "MDMA and related
compounds display unique psychoactive properties,
acting as a stimulant and inducing feelings of empathy....
Forensic investigations performed at our institute
showed significant blood levels of MDMA, MDEA and
MDA in samples drawn from people suspected of driving
under the influence of psychoactive drugs.... Our
study shows that because of the variable composition
of ecstasy tablets, unpredictable types and amounts
of drugs may be taken by MDMA misusers. Moreover,
there is considerable concern that traffic accidents
may be caused by MDMA-abusers."
4.4 IDMU"s
1998 and 1999 drug user surveys found the overall
accident rate for the survey respondents as a whole
to be 0.608 per 100,000km (898 accidents in 147.8
million km), close to the national average. Frequency
of ecstasy use was assessed as experimental (less
than 10 times), occasional, regular, and daily. The
accident rate for all ecstasy users was slightly (9%)
higher than the group as a whole at 0.664, although
regular users of Ecstasy showed a significantly increased
risk of accidents (+50% in mean accidents) and a significantly
increased accident rate (0.849) compared to non-users
(+58%) or all users of other drugs (+40%). However
the extent to which this increase reflects the effects
of the drug itself, rather than the ecstasy using
lifestyle (more likely to be driving in the small
hours of the morning) is unclear.
Ecstasy & Accidents (IDMU 1998-1999
data)
|
Freq
|
No Drivers
|
Mean accidents*
|
Total Number
|
Mean km/ 5yrs
|
Total km/5yrs
|
Total accids
|
Accident Rate
|
% users drive
|
Never
|
1020
|
0.39
|
1395
|
53200.93
|
74215297
|
398
|
0.536
|
73.1
|
Exp
|
288
|
0.43
|
367
|
50140.52
|
18401571
|
124
|
0.673
|
78.5
|
Occ
|
278
|
0.36
|
358
|
56314.91
|
20160738
|
100
|
0.496
|
77.7
|
Reg
|
350
|
0.59
|
477
|
50973.81
|
24314507
|
207
|
0.849*
|
73.4
|
Daily
|
5
|
0.60
|
8
|
50600
|
404800
|
3
|
0.741
|
62.5
|
Ex-users
|
122
|
0.36
|
161
|
55294.18
|
8902363
|
44
|
0.493
|
75.8
|
Total
|
1121
|
0.46
|
1492
|
52054.68
|
77665583
|
516
|
0.664
|
75.1
|
4.5 The study also asked respondents
whether they had had accidents under the influence
of particular drugs. Of 245 such accidents reported,
29 (12%) involved ecstasy (7 alone and 22 in combination
with other drugs). Taking into account the probability
that drivers would be under the influence of the drug
at any particular time (a function of the incidence
of use and frequency of use), this rate was 80% higher
than should have been expected.
4.6 In the 1994 IDMU study, heavy
polydrug use was associated with a significantly higher
level of accidents.
5 Culpability
analysis studies
5.1 Crouch
et al considered that in 50 out of 56 cases where
drugs or alcohol were found, these contributed to
the accident, however it is unclear to what extent
drugs other than alcohol were increased culpability.
5.2 An
Australian study of Drummer, investigated over 1000
accidents, using risk analysis to compare the relative
accident risks of alcohol, cannabis and other drugs,
finding alcohol (p<0.0001) and multiple drug use
(p<.05) to significantly increase the accident
risk, whereas cannabinoids reduced the risk ratio
to a degree which approached statistical significance
(p=.065). Stimulants (amphetamine, cocaine and ecstasy)
were present in 3.3% of cases, with a slightly increased
level of culpability - an odds ratio of 2.0 (range
0.7-5.6). Although this result did not approach statistical
significance, such an increase cannot be ruled out
due to the low incidence of stimulant positives.
6 Ecstasy
Pharmacokinetics & Drug-Testing
6.1 Pharmacokinetics
is the study of the time course of how drugs are distributed
in the body, how long the effects last and how such
effects relate to drug tests.
6.2 Detection
times for MDMA depend on the bodily fluid and analytical
method used, Gombos quotes 1-3 days using radioimmunoassay
screening and GCMS confirmation, with a GCMS cutoff
level of 200ng/ml. Studying human volunteers, Mas
et al reported "Elimination half-life
was 8.6 h and 7.7 h for high (125mg) and low (75mg)
MDMA doses, respectively."
6.3 Crifasi
et al reported MDMA levels in a na"ve user following
a fatal accident "The concentrations
of MDMA in clotted blood, sodium fluoride-potassium
oxalate anticoagulated blood, vitreous humor, and
urine were 2.32 mg/L, 2.14 mg/L, 1.11 mg/L, and 118.8
mg/L, respectively. The concentrations of the metabolite
MDA were less than 0.25 mg/L in blood and vitreous,
and 3.86 mg/L in the urine."
6.4 Fallon
et al noted the differential plasma half-lives of
stereoscopic enantiomers R-MDMA (5.8 +/- 2.2 h) and
S-MDMA (3.6 +/- 0.9 h), when a racemic mixture was
administered, and noted "Mathematical
modeling of plasma enantiomeric composition vs sampling
time demonstrated the applicability of using stereochemical
data for the prediction of time elapsed after drug
administration." In humans, Moore
et al reported "These data indicate
that S(+)-MDMA is metabolized and eliminated faster
than R(-)-MDMA.". In rats given subcutaneous
or intravenous injections of 20-40mg/kg, Fitzgerald
et al reported "The average half-life
(+/- SD) for all dosing groups was 2.5 +/- 0.8 h for
(-)-(R)-MDMA and 2.2 +/- 0.8 h for (+)-(S)-MDMA."
The same team had earlier noted "This
may be significant since it has been shown that the
S(+) isomer of MDMA is the more neurotoxic isomer
of the racemic drug of abuse MDMA. "
6.5 A
hair testing study by Nakahara et al indicated MDMA
to be rapidly incorporated into dark hair, and remain
for a prolonged period, but hardly any incorporation
into white hair. Nonetheless, Kikura et al considered
"a hair sample is a good specimen
for the confirmation of retrospective use of methylenedioxyamphetamines.
"
6.6 Passive
Exposure & False Positives Passive inhalation
of smoked cocaine, heroin or cannabis can result in
a false-positive test result. However as Ecstasy is
taken orally, the only circumstances where passive
exposure is likely to become an issue is where a drink
has been spiked or a person otherwise administered
the drug without knowledge or consent.
6.7 I
am not aware of any studies as to substances which
could produce false positive test results for MDMA,
although it is theoretically possible that some herbal
or medicinal preparations containing precursor chemicals
could be converted within the body to active substances.
6.8 Summary
- Drug Testing for Ecstasy Blood or urine
tests can remain positive for 1-3 days following an
individual dose of ecstasy, and up to 4-5 days following
cessation among chronic heavy users, or where high
doses are used.
6.9 If
a test to determine ecstasy intoxication is required
(as in driving cases), rather than just to determine
whether an individual has used at some time in the
near to medium-term past (e.g. for employment and
rehabilitation purposes), an assay of MDMA, and other
markers (e.g. stereoisomers and MDA) is required from
blood or saliva samples.
6.10 Presence
of MDMA in urine may indicate use of ecstasy in the
previous 1-7 days, however presence of drugs in urine
provides only evidence of past exposure, and cannot
provide evidence of a person being under the influence
of a drug at the time the sample was taken, or at
any particular point in the past.
7 Summary
- Ecstasy and Driving
7.1 As
a stimulant, the effects of ecstasy on psychomotor
performance are relatively limited, with few differences
in test scores (e.g. reaction time, vigilance) between
users and non-users. However cognitive function, particularly
memory can be severely impaired not only in intoxicated
users, but also in regular users currently abstinent
from the drug, most notably memory where long-term
deficits have been recorded.
7.2 Our
own studies suggest regular Ecstasy use to be associated
with higher risk of accidents among drug-using drivers,
however this may result from the greater likelihood
of ecstasy users to driving in the small hours of
the morning when they would otherwise be fatigued
in any event.
7.3 The
culpability analysis studies from Australia neither
confirm nor rule out a role for stimulant in accident
causation, as the number of stimulant positive accident
victims from which conclusions are drawn fails to
achieve statistical significance, despite large initial
sample populations. By contrast, alcohol and benzodiazepines
were associated with significantly increased rates
of culpability.
7.4 However,
our own studies provide evidence to support the proposition
that ecstasy use may be a significant causal factor
in road traffic accidents, as regular ecstasy users
have reported significantly higher accident rates
than non-users or users of other drugs. Users with
heavy poly-drug habits, or involvement in the wholesale
drugs trade, may be more likely to be involved in
road accidents than other drug users. This may result
from a greater propensity to risk-taking behaviour
in such individuals.
7.5 Further
research is required into the effects of ecstasy on
driving simulator performance and in actual driving
situations, as no controlled published studies appear
available.
7.6 The
presence of levels of ecstasy within urine or blood
can not be presumed to indicate that a person is under
the influence of the drug at the time of the test,
or at any specific time beforehand, or to establish
recent use, intoxication, or impairment. McBay concluded
thus:
"The relationship of specific blood drug levels
to driving impairment has not been established for
drugs other than alcohol, except in cases of extreme
doses that may be expected to produce gross impairment.
Thus even though a blood-drug level may be determined
it is often not clear what it means in impairment".
|
All contents of this web
site & any links to other sites etc, is for educational
& research purposes. IDMU at no time seeks to encourage
illegal activities. All sections of this site and its contents
are protected under copyright laws. © IDMU
Ltd 1994 - 2008
|
|
|
|